Less is more! Human brains struggle to subtract, scientists say

Human brains struggle to take ideas or elements away from designs, such as a bricks in blueprint of a building – and instead tend to add more, a new study reveals. 

In experiments, participants tasked with making a Lego Duplo building stronger tended to add more bricks rather than take them away.

In another computerised task, people tended to add coloured squares to a geometrical pattern to make it symmetrical, rather than remove the squares. 

This mysterious way of thinking may innately bias the way architects design buildings, or could even add unnecessary expenses to governments and councils by adding departments rather than taking them away. 

The study has been led by Professor Gabrielle Adams and Professor Leidy Klotz, psychology researchers at the University of Virginia. 

‘Improving objects, ideas or situations – whether a designer seeks to advance technology, a writer seeks to strengthen an argument or a manager seeks to encourage desired behaviour– requires a mental search for possible changes,’ they say in their research paper.  

‘We investigated whether people are as likely to consider changes that subtract components from an object, idea or situation as they are to consider changes that add new components.

‘We show that people systematically default to searching for additive transformations, and consequently overlook subtractive transformations.’ 

Professor Klotz said he was initially intrigued when building structures from Lego bricks with his two-year-old son. 

‘We were building a Lego bridge, and the two columns on the bridge, one was longer than the other one. 

If you’d been tasked with making this structure stable, would you have added a brick to the left column, or taking one away from the right column? 

‘To make the bridge level I turned around to add a block to the shorter column and by the time I had turned around [my son] had just removed a block from the longer column.’ 

He realised that the human brain may be prone to making improvements by adding more elements, rather than taking them away. 

Another example can be seen in the pedal-less balance bikes used by young children. 

These pedal-less bikes are a recent invention, and let the young riders push along the ground with their feet, almost like a rider on skateboard, rather than using stabilisers. 

‘If you’ve seen a little two-year-old going on these things it just makes you disappointed that you didn’t have it when you were a kid – but the breakthrough was just taking away the peddles.’ 

A small boy riding a pedal-less bike. Generations of people have learnt to ride a bike with the helpful addition of stabilisers - but could our learning to ride have been more pleasurable if designers had just taken away the pedals?

A small boy riding a pedal-less bike. Generations of people have learnt to ride a bike with the helpful addition of stabilisers – but could our learning to ride have been more pleasurable if designers had just taken away the pedals?  

For their study, the researchers conducted experiments involving 1,153 participants to looked at how people respond to a diverse array of problems.

These problems ranged from solving a geometrical puzzle, stabilising a Lego structure and improving a miniature golf course. 

In the Duplo task, 197 participants could stabilise the top platform of the structure so it could hold a masonry brick above the head of an action figure. 

They could do this by either adding new supports to reinforce the single corner block or by removing the corner block and letting the platform sit perfectly flat on the layer below. 

Participants earned $1 for successful completion, but adding Lego bricks cost money – 10 cents each. 

Pictured is one of the stimuli used in the experiments - a Lego Duplo structure. A toy action figure (image removed for reasons of copyright) stood at the height marked on the white paper. Participants could stabilise the top platform of the Lego structure so it could hold a masonry brick above the head of the action figure by adding new supports to reinforce the single corner block or by removing the corner block and letting the platform sit flush on the layer below

Pictured is one of the stimuli used in the experiments – a Lego Duplo structure. A toy action figure (image removed for reasons of copyright) stood at the height marked on the white paper. Participants could stabilise the top platform of the Lego structure so it could hold a masonry brick above the head of the action figure by adding new supports to reinforce the single corner block or by removing the corner block and letting the platform sit flush on the layer below

Half of the participants were told ‘Each piece that you add costs 10 cents’, while the other half of participants were told ‘Each piece that you add costs 10 cents but removing pieces is free’.  

Most of the participants added bricks for support, the researchers found – unless the researchers had reminded them that they could add or remove bricks.

‘When we reminded participants that removing pieces is free, participants were much more likely to remove pieces from the structure than when we didn’t remind them,’ said Professor Adams. 

Follow-up experiments suggest that subtractive changes are cognitively less accessible to people, and so the default strategy becomes to add things. 

The authors conclude that this may be one reason why people struggle to mitigate problems such as overburdened schedules, institutional red tape and damaging impacts on the planet. 

The study has been published today in Nature and is accompanied by a separate piece of commentary by Heeyoung Yoon and Tom Meyvis – researchers who were not involved with the study.