Parrots are capable of understanding and acting on the balance of probabilities

Parrots are capable of understanding and acting on the balance of probabilities to predict future events ‘just like humans’, a new study claims. 

Researchers from the University of Auckland in New Zealand worked with a kea parrot and found she could combine data and predict uncertain future events.

The grasp of statistics was thought to be unique to great ape species, such as humans, gorillas and orangutans but this research suggests that isn’t the case.

The discovery revealed that as well as being able to talk, the birds outperform monkeys and human children in basic maths.

Scroll down for video 

Researchers from the University of Auckland in New Zealand worked with a Kea parrot and found she could combine data and predict uncertain future events

The study mirrored previous trials in primates and human babies, using the same tests on the parrot - the Kea performed better than the babies

The study mirrored previous trials in primates and human babies, using the same tests on the parrot – the Kea performed better than the babies

Tests on the Kea, a parrot species from New Zealand, showed they work out odds to choose which hand a token is in – in order to get a food reward.

Researchers found the parrots combine all the available data including where it is picked from – and who is doing it – to make their decision.

‘They can predict uncertain future events by filling in the gaps from incomplete information,’ said lead author Dr Amalia Bastos.

The study mirrored previous trials in primates and human babies and using the same tests on the parrot they found the Kea performed better than babies and monkeys.

‘If you imagine I am placing my hand into a jar with mostly blue sweets and a few yellow and I take something from that jar, but you can’t see what is in my hand, you might guess I have taken a blue one,’ said Bastos.

The biologist, an Oxford graduate now based at the University of Auckland, said kea can do the same.

In a series of tests their numbers were varied in two transparent jars, as an experimenter offered them one from each, concealed in a closed fist

In a series of tests their numbers were varied in two transparent jars, as an experimenter offered them one from each, concealed in a closed fist

The birds had to opt for a hand by tapping on it. They almost always preferred tokens from jars with a higher frequency of black ones

The birds had to opt for a hand by tapping on it. They almost always preferred tokens from jars with a higher frequency of black ones

Her team trained six parrots – named Blofeld, Bruce, Loki, Neo, Plankton and Taz – to associate black and orange tokens with a food treat, or nothing, respectively.

In a series of tests their numbers were varied in two transparent jars, as an experimenter offered them one from each, concealed in a closed fist.

The birds had to opt for a hand by tapping on it and they almost always preferred tokens from jars with a higher frequency of black ones.

This meant they were more likely to win them the tasty snack.

Even when horizontal barriers were placed inside, altering the proportion of rewarding tokens they could see above, the kea detected the change. 

They selected the jar with the best chance of accessible rewarding tokens.

Dr Bastos said: ‘They were choosing the hand based on the probability of it containing a black token, something only seen before in great apes.

‘Kea have a complex social structure where many live in a group. They come and go as they please. They need to remember the identities of individuals.’

Kea also preferred those from an experimenter who had previously demonstrated a ‘bias’ towards offering a higher number of black tokens.

‘We were very surprised to find kea can use social cues, even from humans, to make these judgements,’ said Bastos.

‘Kea were looking at the biased experimenter understanding they had a preference for a particular type of token, and then selecting this person even when both experimenters had the same populations of tokens.’

This is another ability that was thought to have been unique to great apes.

‘Therefore, just like infants and the great apes, kea made statistical inferences using relative rather than absolute quantities,’ Dr Bastos said.

Previously only humans and other primates have been found to be capable of true statistical inference. The kea is a particularly intelligent parrot

Previously only humans and other primates have been found to be capable of true statistical inference. The kea is a particularly intelligent parrot

It is usually olive-green with a brilliant orange under its wings. It is the only alpine parrot in the world and eats an omnivorous diet

It is usually olive-green with a brilliant orange under its wings. It is the only alpine parrot in the world and eats an omnivorous diet

Parrot brains are very similar to those of primates. Recent research has found they have an enlarged region of grey matter known as the medial spiriform nuclei

Parrot brains are very similar to those of primates. Recent research has found they have an enlarged region of grey matter known as the medial spiriform nuclei

WHAT IS A KEA PARROT? 

The Kea is a large alpine parrot native to the South Island of New Zealand.

It is usually olive-green with a brilliant orange under its wings.

It is the only alpine parrot in the world and eats an omnivorous diet.

It mainly eats roots, leaves, berries, nectar and insects.

It is polygamous with one male having multiple female partners.

They are social creatures living in groups of up to 13 birds. 

It is listed as endangered and there are thought to be only 5,000 in the wild. 

‘They integrated social and physical information into their predictions, using their knowledge of physical barriers and the bias of a sampler, to override predictions based purely on relative probabilities.

‘I didn’t expect them to perform quite as well as chimpanzees do. I cannot wait to see what kea do next. They keep surprising us!’

Previously only humans and other primates have been found to be capable of true statistical inference. 

The kea is a particularly intelligent parrot, Bastos said.

‘They are great fun to work with. They are some of the most intelligent birds I have ever met and they all have amusing personalities.’

She added: ‘Showing this type of complex, higher order cognitive process in a bird may help us to understand the evolutionary history of statistical inference.’ 

It is famously inquisitive - which has led to it becoming endangered in recent years. There are fewer than 5,000 left

It is famously inquisitive – which has led to it becoming endangered in recent years. There are fewer than 5,000 left

Parrot brains are very similar to those of primates. Recent research has found they have an enlarged region of grey matter known as the medial spiriform nuclei. 

It acts as an information superhighway providing them with sophisticated problem solving skills – which include using tools.

They can communicate desires, count, add and subtract. Remarkably, they can even understand the concept of zero.

The kea is the world’s only alpine parrot. About 19 inches long, it is mostly olive green with a brilliant orange under its wings. It has a large, curved beak.

It is famously inquisitive – which has led to it becoming endangered in recent years. There are fewer than 5,000 left.

Known for a trusting nature, they often approach passers by and happily gobble up junk food – but gorging on ice cream and chips left over by hikers – or sometimes fed directly to the birds – can end up killing them.  

The research has been published in the journal Nature Communications.

WHAT IS THE MIRROR TEST OF ANIMAL INTELLIGENCE?

The mirror test was developed by psychologist Gordon Gallup Jr in 1970 as a method for determining whether a non-human animal has the ability of self-recognition.

It’s also known as the ‘mark test’ or ‘mirror self-recognition test’ (MSR).

When conducting the mirror test, scientists place a visual marking on an animal’s body, usually with scentless paints, dyes, or stickers. 

They then observe what happens when the marked animal is placed in front of a mirror. 

The researchers compare the animal’s reaction to other times when the animal saw itself in the mirror without any markings on its body.

Animals that pass the mirror test will typically adjust their positions so that they can get a better look at the new mark on their body, and may even touch it or try to remove it. 

They usually pay much more attention to the part of their body that bears a new marking.

Even if an animal doesn’t pass the test, they may still have interesting reactions to their reflections.

Many species respond aggressively, or even show affectionate behaviour. In such cases, it might be that the animal mistakes its reflection for another of its kind. This can lead to some amusing sights for human observers.

Humans are able to pass the mirror test when they are around 18 months old. But how do other animals fare?

Currently, a number of animal species have passed the mirror test. Not all individuals of each species pass, but many do. 

Animals that have passed the test include: 

  • Asian Elephants
  • The Great Apes
  • Bottlenose Dolphins
  • Orca Whales
  • Eurasian Magpies